African giant rats trained to sniff out illegal wildlife products

S
stenews
Wed, Oct 30, 2024 5:46 PM

African giant rats trained to sniff out illegal wildlife products
Frontiers, Phys.org
October 30, 2024

See link
https://phys.org/news/2024-10-giant-rats-illegal-wildlife-sniffing.html
for photos.

In the past, African giant pouched rats have learned to detect explosives
and the tuberculosis-causing pathogen. Now, a team of researchers have
trained these rats to pick up the scent of pangolin scales, elephant ivory,
rhino horn, and African blackwood. These animals and plants are listed as
threatened and at high danger of extinction.

"Our study shows that we can train African giant pouched rats to detect
illegally trafficked wildlife, even when it has been concealed among other
substances," said Dr. Isabelle Szott, a researcher at the Okeanos
Foundation, and first co-author of the study published in Frontiers in
Conservation Science.

"The rats also continued to detect the wildlife targets after not
encountering that species for a long period," added first co-author Dr.
Kate Webb, an assistant professor at Duke University.

The research for the present study was conducted at APOPO, a
Tanzania-based, non-profit organization that provides low-tech,
cost-efficient solutions to pressing humanitarian challenges.

Ratting Out Wildlife Trafficking
The rats—Kirsty, Marty, Attenborough, Irwin, Betty, Teddy, Ivory, Ebony,
Desmond, Thoreau, and Fossey; some of them named after conservationists and
advocates against wildlife trafficking—underwent several training stages.
During indication training, the rats learned to hold their noses for
several seconds in a hole in which the target scent was placed. When they
correctly performed this 'nose poke' they were rewarded with flavored
rodent pellets.

In the next step, the rats were introduced to non-target odors. These
included electric cables, coffee beans, and washing powder—objects that are
frequently used to mask the scent of wildlife in real-life trafficking
operations. "During the discrimination stage, rats learn to only signal the
odors of the wildlife targets, while ignoring non-targets," Szott said.

The rats were also trained to remember smells. At the end of their
retention training, they were re-introduced to scents they'd not
encountered for five and eight months, respectively. Despite months of
non-exposure, the rats showed perfect retention scores, suggesting that
their cognitive retention performance resembles that of dogs.

By the end of the training, eight rats were able to identify four commonly
smuggled wildlife species among 146 non-target substances.

In real life settings, rats will be able to pull a small ball attached at
the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way rats will be
able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. Credit: APOPO

Rats in Action
"Existing screening tools are expensive and time intensive and there is an
urgent need to increase cargo screening. APOPO's rats are cost-efficient
scent detection tools. They can easily access tight spaces like cargo in
packed shipping containers or be lifted up high to screen the ventilation
systems of sealed containers," Szott explained.

The next step, the scientists said, is to develop ways for the rats to work
within ports through which smuggled wildlife is trafficked. For this
purpose, the rats will be outfitted with custom-made vests. With their
front paws, they will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of
their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way, rats will be able to
alert their handlers when they detect a target.

"The vests are a great example of developing hardware that could be useful
across different settings and tasks, including at a shipping port to detect
smuggled wildlife," said Webb.

This proof-of-principle study demonstrates that rats can successfully
identify trafficked wildlife. This does not mean that it comes without
limitations, the researchers said. For example, the study was conducted in
a controlled environment, which is not reflective of the settings in which
wildlife is commonly trafficked or screened by scent-detection animals.

To deploy rats for this task, new methods need to be developed, the
researchers pointed out.

"Wildlife smuggling is often conducted by individuals engaged in other
illegal activities, including human, drug, and arms trafficking. Therefore,
deploying rats to combat wildlife trafficking may assist with the global
fight against networks that exploit humans and nature," concluded Webb.

https://phys.org/news/2024-10-giant-rats-illegal-wildlife-sniffing.html

African giant rats trained to sniff out illegal wildlife products Frontiers, Phys.org October 30, 2024 See link <https://phys.org/news/2024-10-giant-rats-illegal-wildlife-sniffing.html> for photos. In the past, African giant pouched rats have learned to detect explosives and the tuberculosis-causing pathogen. Now, a team of researchers have trained these rats to pick up the scent of pangolin scales, elephant ivory, rhino horn, and African blackwood. These animals and plants are listed as threatened and at high danger of extinction. "Our study shows that we can train African giant pouched rats to detect illegally trafficked wildlife, even when it has been concealed among other substances," said Dr. Isabelle Szott, a researcher at the Okeanos Foundation, and first co-author of the study published in Frontiers in Conservation Science. "The rats also continued to detect the wildlife targets after not encountering that species for a long period," added first co-author Dr. Kate Webb, an assistant professor at Duke University. The research for the present study was conducted at APOPO, a Tanzania-based, non-profit organization that provides low-tech, cost-efficient solutions to pressing humanitarian challenges. Ratting Out Wildlife Trafficking The rats—Kirsty, Marty, Attenborough, Irwin, Betty, Teddy, Ivory, Ebony, Desmond, Thoreau, and Fossey; some of them named after conservationists and advocates against wildlife trafficking—underwent several training stages. During indication training, the rats learned to hold their noses for several seconds in a hole in which the target scent was placed. When they correctly performed this 'nose poke' they were rewarded with flavored rodent pellets. In the next step, the rats were introduced to non-target odors. These included electric cables, coffee beans, and washing powder—objects that are frequently used to mask the scent of wildlife in real-life trafficking operations. "During the discrimination stage, rats learn to only signal the odors of the wildlife targets, while ignoring non-targets," Szott said. The rats were also trained to remember smells. At the end of their retention training, they were re-introduced to scents they'd not encountered for five and eight months, respectively. Despite months of non-exposure, the rats showed perfect retention scores, suggesting that their cognitive retention performance resembles that of dogs. By the end of the training, eight rats were able to identify four commonly smuggled wildlife species among 146 non-target substances. In real life settings, rats will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way rats will be able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. Credit: APOPO Rats in Action "Existing screening tools are expensive and time intensive and there is an urgent need to increase cargo screening. APOPO's rats are cost-efficient scent detection tools. They can easily access tight spaces like cargo in packed shipping containers or be lifted up high to screen the ventilation systems of sealed containers," Szott explained. The next step, the scientists said, is to develop ways for the rats to work within ports through which smuggled wildlife is trafficked. For this purpose, the rats will be outfitted with custom-made vests. With their front paws, they will be able to pull a small ball attached at the chest of their vest, which emits a beeping sound. This way, rats will be able to alert their handlers when they detect a target. "The vests are a great example of developing hardware that could be useful across different settings and tasks, including at a shipping port to detect smuggled wildlife," said Webb. This proof-of-principle study demonstrates that rats can successfully identify trafficked wildlife. This does not mean that it comes without limitations, the researchers said. For example, the study was conducted in a controlled environment, which is not reflective of the settings in which wildlife is commonly trafficked or screened by scent-detection animals. To deploy rats for this task, new methods need to be developed, the researchers pointed out. "Wildlife smuggling is often conducted by individuals engaged in other illegal activities, including human, drug, and arms trafficking. Therefore, deploying rats to combat wildlife trafficking may assist with the global fight against networks that exploit humans and nature," concluded Webb. https://phys.org/news/2024-10-giant-rats-illegal-wildlife-sniffing.html